6.21外刊打卡2

JohnsonTo master a language, start learning it early

New evidence suggests a drop-off in results after the age of 17

Print edition | Books and arts

May 10th 2018

THOSE who want to learn a foreign language, or want their children to, often feel they are racing against the clock. People seem to get worse at languages as they age. Children often learn their first without any instruction, and can easily become multilingual with the right exposure. But the older people get, the harder it seems to be. Witness the rough edges on the grammar of many immigrants even after many years in their new countries.

Scientists mostly agree that children are better language learners, but do not know why. Some posit biological factors. Is it because young brains have an extreme kind of plasticity? Or, as Steven Pinker, a Harvard psychologist, argues, an instinct for language-learning specifically, which fades as the brain ages and (in evolutionary terms) is no longer needed? Others think children have special environments and incentives, not more conducive brains. They have a strong motivation to communicate with caregivers and imitate peers, and are not afraid of making mistakes in the way adults are.

Get our daily newsletter

Upgrade your inbox and get our Daily Dispatch and Editor‘s Picks.

Latest stories

  • Retail sales, producer prices, wages and exchange rates

    3 hours ago

  • Foreign reserves

    3 hours ago

  • Why tipping in America is up for debate

    The Economist explains11 hours ago

  • America’s trade spats are rattling markets

    Graphic detail15 hours ago

  • Colombia’s new president is burdened with political baggage

    Americas15 hours ago

  • Donald Trump signs an executive order to stop family separations

    Democracy in America16 hours ago

See more

Some believe any “critical period” may only apply to the sounds of a foreign tongue. Adults struggle with accents: eight decades after immigrating to America and four after serving as secretary of state, Henry Kissinger still sounds fresh off the boat from Fürth—in what is nevertheless elaborately accurate English. (An alternative explanation, runs a joke about Mr Kissinger, is that he never listens.)

But grammar is different, and some researchers have reckoned that adults, with their greater reasoning powers, are not really at a disadvantage relative to children. One study found that when adults and children are exposed to the same teaching materials for a new language for several months, the adults actually do better. Most such research has had to rely on small numbers of subjects, given the difficulty of recruiting them; it is hard to know how meaningful the results are.

Now a large new study led by Joshua Hartshorne of Boston College (with Mr Pinker and Joshua Tenenbaum as co-authors) has buttressed the critical-period hypothesis. The study ingeniously recruited 670,000 online test-takers by framing the exercise as a quiz that would guess the participants’ native language or dialect. This made it a viral hit. The real point was to test English-learners’ knowledge of tricky bits of grammar, and to see how this correlates with the age at which their studies began.

Do younger beginners do better because their earlier start gave them more learning time, or because they learned faster in early years? It can be hard to tease apart these two questions. But testing a huge amount of data against a number of possible learning curves allowed Mr Hartshorne to do precisely that. Many previous researchers had posited a drop-off at around puberty. The new study found it to be rather later, just after 17.

Despite that later cut-off, learners must begin at around ten if they are to get to near-native fluency. If they start at, say, 14, they cannot accumulate enough expertise in the critical period. Unfortunately, 14 or so is precisely when many students, especially in America, are first introduced to a new language. (Even worse, this is an age when children are acutely sensitive to embarrassment in front of peers.)

Children who start at five don’t do noticeably better than those who start at ten over their lifetimes. But there is still reason to begin in the first years of school, as in Denmark and Sweden. Because mastery takes a long time—perhaps 30 years until improvement ceases—those who begin at five and are obliged to read and write English at university will by then have made much more progress than those who took the plunge at ten, even if their level is roughly the same by 40.

The existence of the critical period is not a reason for anyone 11 or older to give up. Some people remain excellent language students into adulthood. And Mr Hartshorne tested some truly subtle features of grammar that take years to master. A language learned even to a lower level can still be extraordinarily useful at work or enjoyable while travelling.

But for policymakers, the implication is clear. Earlier is better. Students outside the English-speaking world will eventually face English in the classroom or at work: they’ll have a better shot if they start younger. As for the Anglophone countries, getting foreign languages into the tender years is a hard sell. Many bureaucrats can hardly see past reading and maths. That is a mistake for many reasons. This study demonstrates one of them.

This article appeared in the Books and arts section of the print edition under the headline "Out of the mouths of babes"

原文地址:https://www.cnblogs.com/wanghui626/p/9210796.html

时间: 2024-08-01 02:59:03

6.21外刊打卡2的相关文章

6.21外刊打卡4

knuckle down 英 [?n?kl daun] 美 [?n?k?l da?n] v. 认输; (向某人) 屈服; 开始认真工作; [例句]The only thing to do was knuckle down and get on with some serious hard work 唯一能做的就是开始努力工作,好好干出点儿成绩来. https://www.economist.com/books-and-arts/2018/05/10/to-master-a-language-st

6.21外刊打卡3

Hedge funds and artificial intelligence 对冲基金与人工智能 商业银行的风险对冲可以分为自我对冲和市场对冲两种情况: 1.所谓自我对冲是指商业银行利用资产负债表或某些具有收益负相关性质的业务组合本身所具有的对冲特性进行风险对冲. 2.市场对冲是指对于无法通过资产负债表和相关业务调整进行自我对冲的风险(又称残余风险),通过衍生产品市场进行对冲. 假如你在10元价位买了一支股票,这个股票未来有可能涨到15元,也有可能跌到7元.你对于收益的期望倒不是太高,更主要的

6.22外刊打卡

<赖世雄讲英语>第26期:政治民意调意_品牌英语口语 - 可可英语http://www.kekenet.com/kouyu/201208/195943.shtml Use this to correct the anounce. for instance, asked !!!. Ownig enormous power is a marvellous thing! There is nothing with gratitution. This is Scientific American's 

敏捷21天打卡-精益产品开发最佳实践 之 “AB测试&quot;

”一个错误的背后往往存在一个正确的假设,对假设的肯定程度越高引发的错误越严重!“ 从上章节中“MVP”中一文中我们了解到,最低限度的可用的产品是MVP的精髓,其目的在于迅速验证产品的可用性及市场用户的反馈:不能市场用户需要一辆汽车,我们给对方一辆自行车,典型的违背市场的原则. 伟人曾说过“实践出真知”,对于软件行业亦是如此,有了最低限度的可用产品,这是就需要收集市场的反馈,根据反馈调整产品策略,A/B测试是众多工具/方法中的一种. “推出一个代步工具.节约用户上下班时间,从而让用户有更多的时间来

敏捷21天打卡-AARRR模型

介绍下AARRR模型,AARRR模型是Acquisition(获取).Activation(活跃).Retention(留存).Revenue(收入).Refer(传播)的缩写,对应了产品生命周期的每个阶段.而无论是那个阶段,都是围绕着中间的用户展开,为用户提供有价值的产品和服务. 获取:对于一款产品,首先要获取用户,即拉新.如果没有新用户,产品就如一潭死水,即为死水,何来繁荣之说. 活跃:产品有了用户之后,就要考虑如何让用户活跃起来.”久别重逢的老友聚会,缅怀过去光辉岁月之时,有人提议创建个群

敏捷21天打卡--用户故事

在远古时代,文字没有出现之前.知识的传承靠口口相传,不管隔了多少代,其准确性很高.文字出现后,我们大脑中的这种技能反而逐步衰退.于是各种软件.各种方法充斥着我们,左右着我们.各位是否也有相同的想法?试想一下,上节课我们讲了什么?每次应允别人的承诺,我们转过头就忘记?比如忘记约会,忘记洗车.忘记写日报? “要想知道栗子的味道,你得咬一口”这个真理朴实,正确.当用户提出一款软件时,我们不要只想着从技术的角度,认为用户不专业,他的需求是错误的.如果他专业,就没我们啥事了.用户大脑中的需求是离散的,不可

敏捷21天打卡--Scrum角色

Scrum团队由一名产品负责人.开发团队.Scrum Master组成.Scrum团队是跨职能的自组织团队,团队成员自己选择如何以最好的方式完成工作,而不是由团队之外的人来指导.理论上来讲,团队拥有完成工作所需的全部技能,不需要依赖团队之外的人. Scrum团队迭代增量式交付产品,通过这种方式最大的获得反馈的机会,增量式交付”完成“产品保证一个可以工作产品的. 产品负责人PO:Product Owner角色定义,产品负责人的职责是将开发团队研发的产品价值最大化,为产品回报率负责,负责维护Prpd

敏捷21天打卡--Scrum活动

Sprint(冲刺)是Scrum的核心,持续时间为一个月或更短的时间,在这个时间内构建一个完成.可用的和潜在可发布的产品增量.在整个开发过程期间,其长度应保持一致,前一个Sprint完成后,新的下一个Sprint紧接着就开始,有点像接力棒的游戏. Sprint计划会议:会议时间不要过长,不要为了会议而会议.通常一个月内的,上线为8小时或更短:依次类推.其主要目的是确定每个参会者都理解会议的目的.敏捷教练要确保会议梳理举行并教导敏捷团队遵守时间盒规则.包含几点:这次做什么?如何完成所选的工作?期间

敏捷21天打卡-敏捷项目管理(终章)

软件项目管理的两大主流管理模式:传统项目管理(预测型项目管理).敏捷项目管理: 传统项目管理(预测型项目管理):瀑布式.部分迭代开发模式,要求在项目一开始,需求足够明确.文档足够规范.迭代过程需求变更越频繁,其对项目造成的遭难往往越大.相信很多IT团队都尝试过,这里不赘述. 敏捷项目管理作为新兴的项目管理模式,简化了传统项目的流程,从繁琐的流程和详尽的文档中解脱出来.但并不代表敏捷不做计划,有很多人的观念“敏捷不做计划”这是错误,否“probacklog.scrum.看板.燃起图.燃尽图.用户故